X Close

Don’t trust the psychologists on coronavirus Many of the responses to Covid-19 come from a deeply-flawed discipline

oldiers and private contractors help to prepare the temporary Nightingale hospital for coronavirus patients. Photo: Stefan Rousseau - WPA Pool/Getty Images

oldiers and private contractors help to prepare the temporary Nightingale hospital for coronavirus patients. Photo: Stefan Rousseau - WPA Pool/Getty Images


March 31, 2020   7 mins

Psychologists haven’t had a great few years. First there was the “replication crisis”, which kicked off in about 2011 and involved the gradual realisation that many of our best-known, most-hyped results couldn’t be repeated in independent experiments. Then there were the revelations that the American Psychological Association, one of the field’s most important professional bodies, had colluded with the US government on its torture programme during the Iraq War, then attempted to cover it up.

Then some of the most famous studies from social psychology’s 1970s heyday fell apart on closer scrutiny. The Stanford Prison Experiment, where people were assigned roles as “prisoners” and “guards”, and the guards ended up treating the prisoners abominably? Probably misreported. The study where “pseudopatients” admitted themselves to psychiatric hospitals, acted entirely sane, and were locked up and medicated regardless? Possibly fraudulent.

Now, psychologists are disgracing themselves anew over the coronavirus.

It started with articles relying on psychological “insights” to downplay the severity of the problem. In early February, social psychologist David DeSteno wrote a piece in the New York Times arguing that people get so caught up with their fear of the virus, they fail to understand that they’re unlikely to get it. Referencing some of his own lab experiments, DeSteno wrote that “…quarantine or monitoring policies can make great sense when the threat is real and the policies are based on accurate data. But the facts on the ground, as opposed to the fear in the air, don’t warrant such actions.”

Two days later, the New York Times’s Interpreter column quoted psychologist Paul Slovic, who noted that “[o]ur feelings don’t do arithmetic very well”, and that focusing on the coronavirus fatalities, and not the “98% or so of people who are recovering from it and may have mild cases” is skewing our judgement. The article argued that our fears, triggered by disturbing reports of “city-scale lockdowns and overcrowded hospitals”, overload our critical faculties, making us overreact to the threat the virus poses. The thought that those city-scale lockdowns and overcrowded hospitals might be a mere month away from the United States didn’t seem to occur.

Further psychological insights were provided by Cass Sunstein, co-author of the best-selling book Nudge, which used lessons from behavioural economics (essentially psychology by another name) that could inform attempts to change people’s behaviour. In an article for Bloomberg Opinion on 28 February (by which point there were over 83,000 confirmed coronavirus cases worldwide), Sunstein wrote that anxiety regarding the coronavirus pandemic was mainly due to something called “probability neglect”.

Because the disease is both novel and potentially fatal, Sunstein reasoned, we suffer from “excessive fear” and neglect the fact that our probability of getting it is low. “Unless the disease is contained in the near future,” he continued, “it will induce much more fear, and much more in the way of economic and social dislocation, than is warranted by the actual risk”.

On 12 March, the day after Italy had announced its 827th death from the virus, the eminent psychologist Gerd Gigerenzer published a piece in Project Syndicate entitled “Why What Does Not Kill Us Makes Us Panic”. It was, to say the least, confused: it opened with an acknowledgement that we don’t know how bad this epidemic could be, but immediately went on to make the case that we’d likely overreact, and failed to consider any opposing arguments.

Gigerenzer’s article discussed the 2009 swine flu pandemic, which affected hundreds of millions but killed relatively few, particularly in Europe, where the fear was nonetheless high. It also likened the coronavirus pandemic to terrorist attacks, which similarly kill small numbers in a relative sense, but — according to Gigerenzer — garner disproportionate attention. If only we could improve our “risk literacy”, Gigerenzer maintained, then we could “approach situations such as the COVID-19 epidemic with a cooler head”.

At the time of writing, Italy’s death toll is now above 10,000, with over 900 fatalties being reported in a single day. The epidemic in the UK has reached 1,000 deaths; in France, 2,000; in Spain, nearly 6,000. New York City is setting up makeshift morgues for the first time since 9/11. The suspiciously uniform daily death figures from Iran might belie a truly devastating epidemic in that country. The world’s economy has largely closed down, as we all anticipate the pandemic getting far worse before it gets better. How could all those psychologists have gotten it so disastrously wrong?

To back up his points about “probability neglect”, Sunstein had referred to a 2001 paper in the journal Psychological Science. It reported three experiments; Sunstein focused on the third one, which included 156 participants, all of whom were undergraduate students reasoning about how much they’d pay to avoid an imaginary electric shock. It’s not a criticism of the scientists to say that this experiment is only tenuously relevant to a global pandemic.

Indeed, alongside talk of the “replication crisis” there’s been discussion of a “generalisability crisis”, with renewed realisation that results from lab experiments don’t necessary generalise to other contexts. A global pandemic of a completely novel virus is, by its very definition, a context never encountered before. So how can we be sure that the results of behavioural science experiments — even those that are based on bigger or more representative samples than 156 undergrads — are relevant to our current situation?

The answer is that we can’t. Exploring the human capacity for bias and irrationality can make for quirky, thought-provoking articles and books that make readers feel smarter (and can build towards a tentative scientific understanding of how the mind works). But when a truly dangerous disease comes along, relying on small-scale lab experiments and behavioural-economic studies results in dreadful misfires like the articles we encountered above.

Although there are many behavioural phenomena that certainly seem relevant to today’s news — bias, sunk costs, the tragedy of the commons — it’s not at all clear how these concepts would be practically applied to do what needs to be done right now: slowing the spread of the disease.

We can even play the psychologists at their own game: there are also biases in the opposite direction to those discussed above. For instance, “exponential growth bias” might mean that our standard ways of reasoning break down in a situation where a threat accelerates like this virus. As observed in an insightful article on Italy’s ongoing horrific experience, there’s also “confirmation bias”, where we seek out evidence that confirms our previous beliefs or desires.

In this case, many were understandably desperate to believe that the virus wouldn’t be too much of a problem, and it seems to have led them to underreact very badly indeed. None of the psychological writers above seemed to consider these opposing biases — and ironically, it might have been because they were biased against thinking about them.

That latter idea is just speculation on my part — which is the whole point. As the psychologist Stefan Schubert has noted, “[i]t’s too easy to make up a just-so-story” about us being biased in one direction or another. “The ‘psychology of X’ (e.g. risk, disease) can only tell us so much about X. We should mostly look at X itself”.

Quite so: the critical task for now is not to try and get inside people’s heads, but to learn from the experience of other countries (like Taiwan, South Korea and Singapore) that have been more successful in containing covid-19; and to build realistic mathematical models of the epidemic, and to pour resources into medical advances — like tests, vaccines, and antivirals — that might help us to track and inhibit the transmission of the virus and prevent its symptoms.

Sunstein himself appears to have undergone something of a Damascene corona-conversion. On 26 March, he wrote another column for Bloomberg Opinion, entitled “This Time the Numbers Show We Can’t Be Too Careful”. In an argument that was directly opposed to his own reasoning of a month earlier, Sunstein suggested that the potentially ruinous effects of the pandemic mean that “the benefits of aggressive social distancing greatly exceed the cost”. Remarkably, he made no reference whatsoever to his previous article on this specific crisis — an article which, as the writer Ari Schulman has pointed out, had said that if you held the view Sunstein now holds, you were the sad victim of a cognitive malfunction.

It’s impossible to know how many people changed the way they thought about the virus after reading one of the misconceived articles above. But a consistent set of articles by credentialed scientists telling people they shouldn’t worry so much about the pandemic was the precise opposite of what was required in a situation where governments would eventually, and rightly, be imploring their citizens to stay at home at all costs. At best, the advice from psychology was unhelpful. At worst, it constituted dangerous misinformation.

And what of those governments? Could the “don’t overreact” advice from behavioural science have led politicians to drag their feet in dealing with the crisis, as some clearly have? Again, it’s hard to say — but it’s very possible that the broad message got through to the decision-makers.

Thankfully, the advice given to the UK government by behavioural scientists, which was published just over a week ago, didn’t contain any minimisation of the risks. In fact, it was — in my view at least — mostly banal and common-sensical, noting for instance that using the word “isolation” might have “negative overtones for older adults”, and that there might be unintended consequences if we closed schools.

Other government behavioural advisors, though, were less circumspect. The Behavioural Insights Team, a consulting company nicknamed the “Nudge Unit”, has been brought in to assist the UK’s response. At first it seemed their focus was on how to encourage handwashing, but there appears to have been mission creep.

For example, the team’s head, David Halpern, was interviewed as a “government coronavirus science advisor” about broad policies of “cocooning” older people. He was also quoted in support of the idea — which might yet seem grievously misguided in hindsight — that social-distancing measures should only be brought in gradually, to avoid people becoming fatigued. After he was reportedly “bollocked” by No.10 a fortnight ago for introducing the unfortunate phrase “herd immunity” into the national conversation, Halpern hasn’t (to my knowledge) been heard from again in public.

And perhaps that’s for the best. As intriguing as many psychological studies are, the vast majority of the insights we’ve gained from our research are simply not ready for primetime — especially in the case of a worldwide emergency where millions of lives are at stake. Much of the useful advice behavioural scientists can give isn’t really based on “science” to any important degree, and is intuitive and obvious.

Where they try to be counter-intuitive — for instance, arguing that people are wrong to find a global pandemic frightening — they simply end up embarrassing themselves, or worse, endangering people by having them make fewer pandemic preparations. This isn’t to say that psychology isn’t useful when it stays in its own lane: it’ll be important to ensure that as many people as possible have access to psychotherapy for the mental-health effects of the pandemic, for instance. But that’s a secondary effect of the virus: my argument here is that psychology can give little reliable counsel about our immediate reaction to the pandemic.

Psychologists should know their limits, and avoid over-stretching results from their small-scale studies to new, dissimilar situations. Decision-makers should, before using psychology research as the basis for policy, know just how weak and contentious so much of it is. And everyone else should stay at home, wash their hands — and beware psychologists bearing advice.

 


Stuart Ritchie is a psychologist and a Lecturer in the Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre at King’s College London

StuartJRitchie

Join the discussion


Join like minded readers that support our journalism by becoming a paid subscriber


To join the discussion in the comments, become a paid subscriber.

Join like minded readers that support our journalism, read unlimited articles and enjoy other subscriber-only benefits.

Subscribe
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

18 Comments
Most Voted
Newest Oldest
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments
Niko Lourotos
Niko Lourotos
4 years ago

This “article” is merely more fear mongering. You make no arguments, besides “If we focus on only the scary statistics, taken completely out of context, we are rightfully scared”. It seems to me that you are simply attempting to justify (your?) cowardice.

You are missing the very point. The point is not that psychologists may be “arguing that people are wrong to find a global pandemic frightening”. The point is that they are perfectly capable of ,maintaining perspective and not becoming frightened – indeed that such behaviour is much more useful.

Nigel Clarke
Nigel Clarke
4 years ago
Reply to  Niko Lourotos

Fear mongering…a bit harsh, read the Groan and watch the BBC for Fear mongering.

Agree that we are perfectly capable of maintaining perspective and not becoming frightened…however, the BBC and the MSM actively encourage fear, division and misinformation…subsequently leading to what we see in the UK right now in regard to CV – confusion and ignorance. Just how they like it…

Gerry Fruin
Gerry Fruin
4 years ago
Reply to  Nigel Clarke

Spot on. The BBC continue to plumb the negative aspect of most issues. Now after Brexit they have found a new crisis to distort and pervert information. The Derbyshire police trying to do a good job becomes world news. Really? Give the government and the Frontline troops a break. Alternatively give a chance to rebut the misinformation been spewed out by the usual celebrity/personalities reading their scripts. Or just switch off.

Juilan Bonmottier
Juilan Bonmottier
4 years ago
Reply to  Gerry Fruin

The Beeb ‘Gender and Identity’ reporting brigade has taken about three seconds to redefine the global pandemic of Coronavirus as really being all about domestic abuse -citing one case of a woman in India and one in the states. I don’t wish at all to belittle these women’s plights but for christ’s sake BBC can’t we have a break from feminist sermonizing for at least the duration of a global pandemic?!

Nigel Clarke
Nigel Clarke
4 years ago

Serminising…not sermonizing. This is Britain not Texas!

Robert Forde
Robert Forde
4 years ago
Reply to  Gerry Fruin

The police were going way beyond their powers, and condemning perfectly reasonable and lawful behaviour because it made their job easier, not because it was law. It would make their job easier if we all stayed at home all the time. It would also make it easier if they didn’t exceed their lawful authority.

Robert Forde
Robert Forde
4 years ago

As a psychologist (albeit retired now) I would have to agree with much of this. Many psychologists are indeed so eager to be “relevant” that they are inclined to overstretch the findings of experiments, especially lab studies. I even wrote a book about it: Bad Psychology, which concentrated on my own field of forensic psychology.

However, Ritchie fails to acknowledge that the so-called “replication crisis” is not especially a feature of psychology. Failures to replicate are widespread in other fields too. He also doesn’t mention that patient compliance (eg, with taking medications) is one of the greatest problems in medicine, and that it is health psychologists who have found ways to increase compliance. Encouraging handwashing is just a current application of this. Yes, the actions are often “commonsense” (whatever that is) but that doesn’t mean people readily carry them out.

There is also no doubt that human judgment is prone to many flaws, and failure to understand the statistical nature of much information is a prime example. These flaws genuinely do cause us to draw false conclusions and misinterpret evidence. That is one reason for the whole Roy Meadows sudden infant death fiasco a few years back. It also affects psychologists as much as other people, though: that is why Ministry of Justice psychologists persisted for years with “risk reduction” programmes for sex offenders which in fact made them worse.

And as for denial, it is psychologists who have illuminated the way this causes overenthusiastic pronouncements about major countries being finished with this virus in three weeks. Such trumpery.

Ray Hall
Ray Hall
4 years ago
Reply to  Robert Forde

thank you for the comment . Please could you let me have a source for your very interesting comment about risk reduction . Certainly I have been unconvinced by some of the programmes of which I heard but this was more gut feeling .
( Occasionally some of the people that I dealt with professionally had been the victims of CSA )

Robert Forde
Robert Forde
4 years ago
Reply to  Ray Hall

It’s been in the news quite a bit since the story first broke in 2017. I summarised and referenced in my book “Bad Psychology” some of the main evidence which existed even before the MOJ’s admission of failure. The paper which forced them to admit the problem is available online at: https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/623876/sotp-report-web-.pdf

Since then there has been a court case taken by one of the researchers who was bullied and intimigated for daring to find the wrong thing: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/5d289244ed915d2feaf5f7ff/Ms_K_Hopkins_V_Secretary_of_State_for_Justice-_jdgt__reasons.pdf
I doubt if the legal action is anything like finished yet.

simonclarke74
simonclarke74
4 years ago

Some very salutary lessons for the discipline no doubt. However, I think the author may be overreaching here when he argues that psychology does not have much to say about issues relating to the pandemic. Why, for example, did panic-buying set in so quickly, and why did people buy so much, of all things, loo roll? What were people thinking and why? Why do some people, in the face of such repeated and consistent warnings to social-distance, flagrantly disregard them, and yet others observe stringently? How could they be better encouraged, without the use of blanket force? How do people cope with social isolation, and how could they cope better (or worse)? Why have some police forces gone overboard with enforcing the regulations, whilst others have been more lax, and how successful are the approaches in terms of enforcing outcomes such as social distancing?

All of these questions are relevant to effective management of the pandemic and all of them, arguably, are within the remit of psychology. Indeed, as one of the posters above has mentioned, there are many real-world health psychology studies that have demonstrated compliance with behavioural outcomes in real-world settings. Whilst I am very critical of the discipline as a whole, especially its (lack of) scientific credentials and efficacy, there are clearly many issues where the discipline could speak meaningfully to. Perhaps the issue here has been which theories and research psychologists and their advisors have paid heed to – studies with low ecological validity, small sample sizes, poor generalisability etc. – as the author here as rightfully pointed out.

Jean Fothers
Jean Fothers
4 years ago

I used to work in hospitals and the opinion I formed from observation was this.
In most hospitals, psychologists and psychiatrists usually wear a white coat when ‘treating’ or consulting with patients, most often those with mental health problems. My opinion is that the purpose of the white coat is simply to allow onlookers to know which is the patient and which is not.

Joe Smith
Joe Smith
4 years ago

Whatever the failings of psychologists I think the cognitive biases they identify do skew people’s perception of risk. It would be interesting to see how availability bias played out if we had saturation TV and media coverage of the annual flu season, daily cases and deaths and hospitals overstretched. Would people stop descending en masse to shopping centres and attending Christmas parties if they knew that during their lifetime they may unknowingly cause a flu death by not social distancing?

pulkit35taneja
pulkit35taneja
4 years ago

As a budding psychologist I percieve that making an extreme judgement about a field or discipline is absolutely wrong when your judgement is based on the few people practicing it in way not appreciated by all and having a higher position name and representing the whole community of psychologist as everyone is a human and can have their baises and different schemas and perspectives about a particular siuation. Considering what the article said, I agree that the psychologist mentioned above may have speculated while forming their judgements and should have focused on a more scientific way of approaching a global situation as it can’t be give single conclusion from past experiment on people. But that doesn’t mean that the discipline has nothing to contribute, on very basic note you contradicting their point is because of high meta cognitive abilities and ability to make sense out things in a better way than other as a human (as psychology explains), and indeed people should worry in such pandemic situation but their is a certain line to it because the way all the world is worried, such prolonged stress can lead to mental problems. So I think not completely but in a way you and those psychologists as well are right but not completely.

saswata
saswata
4 years ago

The article makes certain sweeping statements based on a few studies that fail to represent the discipline in its entirety. Framing may lead to cognitive biases, which may distort our risk perception. However, individuals will react differently to varying levels of risk perception. Governments have responded differently based on their risk perception and resource endowment – the ability to contain the spread of the disease. Lastly, viewing a two per cent death rate as small or large is based on how much one values life.

Fraser Bailey
Fraser Bailey
4 years ago

I suspect that most of us don’t trust the psychologists on coronavirus or, indeed, anything else.

Robert Forde
Robert Forde
4 years ago
Reply to  Fraser Bailey

Psychologists have shown that they can be trusted so long as they stick to the evidence. Behaviour exists, and like anything else that exists it can be studied scientifically. Where psychologists go wrong, IMO, (and I say this as a member of the profession since 1971) is when they depart from the evidence, and start giving “professional” opinions which are little more than personal biases or folklore presented as truths. Arguably, one could say the same of all scientists. One problem is that there are many therapists, pundits, etc, who are understood to be psychologists but who actually aren’t.

James Billot
James Billot
4 years ago

A statement from the British Psychological Society:

“We were disappointed to read the ‘Don’t trust the psychologists on coronavirus’ piece, and have written a response on what psychologists are doing to positively contribute to the UK government response. This is published on our website here (https://www.bps.org.uk/news…, and we would ask that you consider including it alongside the original article.”

robagnew
robagnew
4 years ago

Bang on.

But in addition to the various crises in psychology, I think this whole awful event is also highlighting the often poor sense of professional boundary/limit of competence that psychologists can tend towards. And it is in those circumstances that the effect of the crises in psychology cannot be identified and accommodated for in the forming of an opinion.

Last night I watched a developmental (research) psychologist, when asked why it was so hard to stay indoors, tell the nation that it was because ‘we need human contact’ and that we should ‘breathe’. My phone blew up with non-psychologist friends asking me if I was watching it. What that psychologist could have said when asked to comment was “Hmmm… not really the area of expertise of a developmental psychologist… what you need is a health psychologist…they specialise in the psychology of health related behaviour…oh you don’t know one…here, let me help you find one”.

But this is not new. In medico-legal work I frequently come across psychologists undertaking assessments for conditions in which they seem to have no particular expertise. In addition between the different professional disciplines of psychology, we seem to really enjoy stepping into other roles. Educational and forensic psychologists doing general diagnostic assessments, clinical psychologists doing highly specialist risk assessment and capacity assessment that should be done by another profession entirely, counselling psychologists undertaking neuropsychological assessments…. all fine if you work in a service where you are supported or supervised by someone who has been qualified in a discipline where that activity forms part of their core professional training. But actually there seem to me to be many psychologists acting as independent experts in areas where they have acquired their knowledge and skills through osmosis. Not through a programme of education and examination that has been ratified by the BPS.

It seems that there is no real requisite for role specificity in psychology and as a result, because there is crossover in some areas, and because you can have a ‘psychological opinion’ on anything, we get psychologists undertaking activities and opining on matters for which they really have no particular knowledge base or skillset. They might do an OK job. But I wonder how many psychologists would be happy to pay a professional, for a professional service, only to find out that actually they were relying on transferable skills and general knowledge.

‘Media’ psychologists do fantastically well from the lack of regulation and consequence for operating outside of their expertise. The public do not generally differentiate between the different expertise of the different types of psychologist; that should be our ethical undertaking as professionals.

The public are not stupid. They know when we are talking rubbish. And I think it is to the detriment of the psychological professions and not in the public interest.