But what about all those false positives? Credit: Christopher Furlong/Getty Images

In ancient Greek mythology, Cassandra was cursed by the gods: she was given the gift of prophecy, but although her prophecies would always be true, no one would believe them. She foresaw that Helenās abduction would lead to the destruction of Troy; she warned of the wooden horse, filled with Greeks, which was the instrument of that destruction. But her warnings went unheeded.
I donāt want to say Iām exactlyĀ like Cassandra. But I do want to say that everyoneĀ is ranting and ravingĀ about false positive ratesĀ at the moment, almost as if they didnāt listen when I was ranting and raving about them back in early April. It all seems to stem back to an articleĀ on Toby Youngās āLockdown Scepticsā site a few days ago, in which the author correctly pointed out that if a test has a 1% false positive rate, that doesnāt mean that only 1% of positives are false.
From there itās sort of spiralled out of control, with, among other people, Julia Hartley-Brewer sayingĀ that āthe Government is planning to lockdown our country again when there is no evidence of a second waveā and John Redwood demandingĀ that āgovernment advisers today need to tell us how they are going to stop false test results distorting the figuresā.
Hereās whatās going on. First, itās absolutely true that the āfalse positive rateā does not on its own tell you the percentage of positive results which are false. Imagine this: youāve got 10,000 people; you have a test that gives you the correct answer 99% of the time (i.e. 99% of people who do have the disease get a positive result, and 99% of people who donāt have the disease get a negative result); and you have a disease that only 0.1% of the population has (which is roughly what the ONS reckonsĀ is the real prevalence of Covid in the population).
So out of the 10,000 people, there are 10 people with the disease. Your test will probably tell you, correctly, that they all have the disease. But it will also give false positives to 1% of the 9,990 people who donāt have the disease. So it will probably tell about 100 entirely healthy people that they have Covid-19. So in this situation, given that a disease that only 0.1% of people have, a positive result on a test with a 1% false positive rate means you only have about a 9% chance of actually having the disease.
This is all very straightforward and exactly what Bayesā theoremĀ tells us. If you get a positive test, your chance of actually having CovidĀ is dependent not just on how accurate (or technically how āspecificā, the technical term for how good it is at avoiding false positives) it is, but also how common the disease is in the population.
What Hartley-Brewer, John Redwood and others are suggesting is that this means that, actually, thereās no real reason to believe that there is a second wave, because the positive test results hugely overstate the real number of cases. I think theyāre wrong about that, and quite crucially wrong. Let me try to explain why.
(Once again, my thanks to the indefatigable Kevin McConway of the Open University for helping me understand this stuff, and once again, any errors are mine, not his.)
Letās start with the least important thing first. The scepticsā argument assumes that the tests are applied to the population entirely at random. Only 0.1% of the population has the disease, so only 0.1% of the people youāre testing have the disease. But thatās obviously false, because the people who are coming forward to get a test usually have some reason to think they might have, or might have been exposed to, the disease. So the figure will probably be higher than the 0.1%-ish prevalence in the population. Even if the true number is only 1%, that makes a huge difference: suddenly, plugging that into our equation above, youād be looking at 50% of positives being false, not 91%*. The risk of false positives is hugely sensitive to quite small changes in the true incidence rate.
We donāt know the true incidence rate of people coming forward to get tested, but we can be confident itās higher than the background rate in the population. That said, it doesnāt change the underlying point, that some number ā perhaps some large number ā of the positive results are false.
But hereās the second point, which is that it doesnāt really matter. The number of positives has increased, and assuming that your testing process hasnāt changed ā that the tests havenāt literally become worse in some way ā then your false positive rate shouldnāt have changed. If your number of positives has gone up, then that must be because the true positives have gone up.
Letās go back to our 10,000 imaginary people and our 99% specific test. If absolutely nobody has the disease at all, then youāll get about 100 false positives. If 1% of people have it, then youāll get about 99. If 10% of people have it, youāll get about 90. Whatever is going on with the disease, whatever the real-world prevalence, the number of false positives wonāt ever make your data show an upward curve.
But if the number of real-world cases goes up, your true positives will go up very quickly. If absolutely no one has it, obviously youāll get zero true positives. If 0.1% of people have it, as we saw, youāll get about 10 true positives. If 1% do, youāll get about 99 (assuming a false negative rate of 1%, which isnāt right, but for simplicity). If 10% do, youāll get about 990. If you ever see your number of positives going up, it must be because the number of real cases is going up, unless your testing regime has changed. And we are seeing the number of cases go up, so, almost certainly, the number of actual cases is going up too.
Admittedly, if the government started doing many times more tests than it had been in previous weeks, youād see the absolute number of false positives go up ā but it seems unlikelyĀ that the problem with our testing regime, at the moment, is that itās expanded too fast. Itās also worth noting that even though we donāt know the true false positive rate ā thereās no perfect standard to judge it against ā we know that fewer than 1% of all the tests the ONS performs come back positive, so the true positive rate cannot be higher than 1%.
And the important thing here is that weāre dealing with an epidemic, and exponential numbers. How fast the number of cases is doubling is much more important than the actual number itself. As Chris Whitty and Patrick Vallance said in their press conference on Monday 21 September, that number appears to be doubling every seven days. If the real number of cases is only half what the tests say, that only makes a weekās difference to how quickly we have to respond to it. We should really remember that from last time around.
Thatās not the only thing, of course. These tests are one strand of evidence, but there are others. Most notably, the ONS survey on which Hartley-Brewer and the others base their 0.1% figure (and therefore their ā91% false positivesā claim) itself shows a significant rise in prevalence. The dataset is here; the estimated percentage of people who had the disease was 0.64% at the end of April, when the survey began (it was probably even higher in March), and dropped as low as 0.03% in late June and early July. But since the end of August, itās risen back up to about 0.1%. So weāre nowhere near the early peaks, but the number has trebled in the last two months, with especially rapid growth since mid-August.
If you want more evidence you can look at the KCL-ZOE Symptom Study, which tracks the number of people reporting Covid-like symptoms, and which also says that thereās been a rapid growth since the end of August, taking us back to about where we were in early June.
Then there areĀ hospitalisations, which, again, are absolutely nowhere near the horrors of early April, when about 3,500 people across the UK were admitted on a single day; but nor is it particularly near the low point of 22 August, when fewer than 70 people were admitted across the whole country. Now itās around 250.
Hospitalisations lag behind infections, and deathsĀ lag behind hospitalisations: deaths in the UK dropped to fewer than 10 a day in early September and reached 22 on 16 September, so we may be about to see a real increase there. (The data there gets a bit complicated, because deaths take some time to be reported ā sometimes months ā so those likely arenāt the final figures. But I think itās reasonable to say, looking at the graph, that the beginnings of a rise are visible. Again, itās nothing like the April-May peak, but I think itās real.)
Is this āevidence of a second waveā, as Hartley-Brewer argues it isnāt? Well, it all depends on what āsecond waveā means. Thereās absolutely no evidence that people are dying, or getting hospitalised, or even getting infected, at the rates they were six months ago. But there isĀ evidence that theyāre getting infected at the rates there were maybe seven months ago. Eight at a push? I donāt think weāre going to go waltzing merrily down the exact same path as we did last time, but we should have learnt by now that exponential curves can go up pretty quickly if you donāt take action.
None of this is to say that the false positive rate complaint is garbage ā Hartley-Brewer, Redwood et al are right to say that the number of positive test results does not, on its own, tell us about the number of actual cases.
And thereās a bigger point, which McConway pointed out to me, which is that you see charts like this one going around, showing the number of test-confirmed cases, and which make it look as though the numbers weāre seeing now are in any way comparable to the numbers back in March and April. But back then, as McConway says, āthe only way you could get a confirmed test in April was if you were in hospital, pretty much.ā
The true peak was many, many times higher than the number of test-confirmed cases; we are now doing hundreds of times as many tests. Itās not enough, but itās loads more than in those hectic early months. So the number of positive results ā false or true ā will be many, many times greater, per actual case in the community, than it was back then. Those sorts of comparisons are silly and spurious, and itās right to say that weāre nowhere near those numbers.
But Iām very wary about being complacent. I was sceptical, a few months back, about claims that locking down a week earlier would have saved half of the lives lost; maybe I was right to be, but even if that was an overstatement, I think most scientists agree that faster action could have saved many thousands of lives. If the curve is going up, thatās what matters, not whether the exact number is 6,000 cases or 3,000.
The Government seems to be taking some sort of action this time, and hopefully it wonāt repeat all the mistakes it made back in March and April. There were plenty of Cassandras back then, rightly warning of the doom to come, and being ignored; hopefully this time theyāll be listened to.
* Thereād be 9,900 people without the disease in your 10,000 tests; your test would give you false positives on 99 of them. But there would be 100 people who actually had the disease, and your test would identify 99 of them correctly. So thatās 50% true and 50% false positives.
Join the discussion
Join like minded readers that support our journalism by becoming a paid subscriber
To join the discussion in the comments, become a paid subscriber.
Join like minded readers that support our journalism, read unlimited articles and enjoy other subscriber-only benefits.
Subscribe